toplogo
登入
洞見 - Human-Computer Interaction - # Trust dynamics in human-autonomy interaction

Predicting Trust Dynamics in Human-Autonomy Interaction: Associations between Personal Characteristics and Trust Dynamics Clusters


核心概念
Distinct trust dynamics clusters (Bayesian decision makers, disbelievers, oscillators) are associated with specific personal characteristics, including masculinity, positive affect, extraversion, neuroticism, intellect, performance expectancy, and high expectations.
摘要

The study examined the relationships between personal characteristics and trust dynamics in human-autonomy interaction. 130 participants performed a simulated surveillance task aided by an automated threat detector.

The key findings are:

  1. Clustering analysis revealed three distinct trust dynamics clusters: Bayesian decision makers (BDMs), disbelievers, and oscillators.

  2. Significant differences were found across the three clusters in seven personal characteristics dimensions:

  • Oscillators had higher scores in masculinity, positive affect, extraversion, and intellect.
  • Disbelievers had higher neuroticism and lower performance expectancy and high expectations.
  1. The three clusters also differed in their behaviors and post-experiment ratings. Disbelievers were least likely to blindly follow the automated recommendations.

  2. A decision tree model was developed to predict a user's trust dynamics cluster based on the seven significant personal characteristics, achieving 70% accuracy.

The study provides a comprehensive understanding of how diverse users exhibit varying trust dynamics and the role of specific personal characteristics as antecedents of trust dynamics in human-autonomy interaction.

edit_icon

客製化摘要

edit_icon

使用 AI 重寫

edit_icon

產生引用格式

translate_icon

翻譯原文

visual_icon

產生心智圖

visit_icon

前往原文

統計資料
The automated threat detector had a reliability level of 62%, 64%, 66%, 68%, or 70%. The tracking score was calculated based on the root mean square error (RMSE) of the tracking task performance. The detection score was calculated based on the detection correctness and response time.
引述
"Disbelievers are characterized by high scores for neuroticism and low scores for performance expectancy and PAS-high expectations." "Oscillators displayed higher scores in masculinity, positive affect, extraversion and intellect."

深入探究

How do the personal characteristics of disbelievers and oscillators influence their trust recovery and calibration after trust violations?

The personal characteristics of disbelievers and oscillators significantly influence their trust recovery and calibration following trust violations. Disbelievers, characterized by high neuroticism and low performance expectancy, tend to exhibit a more pessimistic outlook towards autonomous systems. Their high levels of neuroticism make them more susceptible to negative emotions, which can hinder their ability to recover trust after a violation. This emotional instability leads to a tendency to rely on their judgment rather than the automated system, resulting in a lower likelihood of trust recovery. Consequently, disbelievers may remain skeptical and maintain low trust levels even after positive interactions, as their expectations of the system's reliability are inherently low. In contrast, oscillators display fluctuating trust dynamics, influenced by their higher scores in positive affect, extraversion, and intellect. These characteristics enable oscillators to experience a broader range of emotional responses, allowing them to recalibrate their trust more dynamically. Their positive affect and extraversion may facilitate quicker recovery from trust violations, as they are more likely to engage with the system and reassess its reliability based on recent interactions. However, the oscillators' tendency to oscillate between high and low trust can lead to inconsistent trust calibration, making it challenging to predict their trust levels accurately. Overall, while disbelievers struggle with trust recovery due to their negative emotional predispositions, oscillators may recover more readily but with significant fluctuations in their trust levels.

What are the potential biases or limitations in the self-reported personal characteristics data collected through surveys?

Self-reported personal characteristics data collected through surveys can introduce several biases and limitations that may affect the validity of the findings. One significant limitation is the potential for social desirability bias, where participants may respond in a manner they believe is more socially acceptable or favorable, rather than providing honest answers. This can lead to inflated scores in traits such as extraversion or positive affect, skewing the results and misrepresenting the true characteristics of the participants. Additionally, recall bias may affect the accuracy of self-reported data, as participants might struggle to accurately remember their past behaviors or feelings, particularly in relation to complex constructs like trust dynamics. This can result in inconsistencies between their reported characteristics and their actual behaviors during the experiment. Another limitation is the reliance on subjective measures, which may not capture the full complexity of personal characteristics. For instance, personality traits are multifaceted and can vary in expression depending on context, yet surveys often reduce these traits to simplified scales. This reduction may overlook nuances that are critical for understanding how personal characteristics influence trust dynamics. Lastly, the sample size and diversity of the participant pool can also impact the generalizability of the findings. If the sample lacks diversity in terms of demographics such as age, gender, or cultural background, the results may not be applicable to broader populations. Therefore, while self-reported surveys are valuable for gathering personal characteristics data, researchers must be cautious of these biases and limitations when interpreting the results.

How can the findings from this study be applied to design trust-aware autonomous systems that can dynamically adapt to users with different trust dynamics?

The findings from this study provide valuable insights for designing trust-aware autonomous systems that can dynamically adapt to users with varying trust dynamics. By understanding the distinct characteristics of trust dynamics clusters—namely, Bayesian decision makers, disbelievers, and oscillators—developers can create systems that tailor their interactions based on the user's trust profile. For disbelievers, who exhibit high neuroticism and low performance expectancy, autonomous systems can implement features that enhance transparency and provide more detailed explanations of their decision-making processes. By offering clear rationales for actions and predictions, these systems can help alleviate the disbelievers' skepticism and gradually build trust over time. Additionally, incorporating feedback mechanisms that allow users to express their concerns or doubts can further engage disbelievers and encourage them to reassess their trust levels. For oscillators, who display fluctuating trust dynamics, systems can be designed to monitor real-time trust levels and adapt their communication strategies accordingly. For instance, if an oscillator's trust dips after a violation, the system could increase its engagement through proactive notifications or reminders of past successes to help recalibrate trust. Furthermore, providing personalized experiences based on the user's emotional state—such as adjusting the tone of communication or the complexity of information presented—can enhance the user's overall experience and foster a more stable trust relationship. Overall, by leveraging the insights gained from the study, designers can create adaptive, trust-aware systems that not only recognize individual differences in trust dynamics but also implement strategies to foster trust recovery and calibration, ultimately leading to more effective human-autonomy interactions.
0
star